HAVING -v- DOING

Separating “sustainability” from pure “survival” leads us to quality-of-life considerations.  Here’s a fascinating piece of research that may change, for the good, how you spend your time and money.

 •       •       •       •      •       •       •       •

 

The Science Of

Why You Should

Spend Your Money On

Experiences, Not Things

 

Jay Cassano

 

You don’t have infinite money. Spend it on stuff that research says makes you happy.

Most people are in the pursuit of happiness. There are economists who think happiness is the best indicator of the health of a society. We know that money can make you happier, though after your basic needs are met, it doesn’t make you that much happier. But one of the biggest questions is how to allocate our money, which is (for most of us) a limited resource.

There’s a very logical assumption that most people make when spending their money: that because a physical object will last longer, it will make us happier for a longer time than a one-off experience like a concert or vacation. According to recent research, it turns out that assumption is completely wrong.

Dr. Thomas Gilovich, a psychology professor at Cornell University who has been studying the question of money and happiness for over two decades says …

 

“One of the enemies of happiness is adaptation.

We buy things to make us happy, and we succeed.

But only for a while.  New things are exciting

to us at first, but then we adapt to them.”

 

So rather than buying the latest iPhone or a new BMW, Gilovich suggests you’ll get more happiness spending money on experiences like going to art exhibits, doing outdoor activities, learning a new skill, or traveling.

Gilovich’s findings are the synthesis of psychological studies conducted by him and others into the Easterlin paradox, which found that money buys happiness, but only up to a point. How adaptation affects happiness, for instance, was measured in a study that asked people to self-report their happiness with major material and experiential purchases.

 

Initially, their happiness with those

purchases was ranked about the same.

 

But over time, people’s satisfaction with the

things they bought went down, whereas

their satisfaction with experiences

they spent money on went up.

 

It’s counterintuitive that something like a physical object that you can keep for a long time doesn’t keep you as happy as long as a once-and-done experience does. Ironically, the fact that a material thing is ever present works against it, making it easier to adapt to. It fades into the background and becomes part of the new normal. But while the happiness from material purchases diminishes over time, experiences become an ingrained part of our identity.  Gilovich says …

 

“Our experiences are a bigger part of ourselves

than our material goods.  You can really like your

material stuff. You can even think that part of your

identity is connected to those things, but nonetheless

they remain separate from you. In contrast, your

experiences really are part of you. We are

the sum total of our experiences.”

 

One study conducted by Gilovich even showed that if people have an experience they say negatively impacted their happiness, once they have the chance to talk about it, their assessment of that experience goes up. Gilovich attributes this to the fact that something that might have been stressful or scary in the past can become a funny story to tell at a party or be looked back on as an invaluable character-building experience.

Another reason is that shared experiences connect us more to other people than shared consumption. You’re much more likely to feel connected to someone you took a vacation with in Bogotá than someone who also happens to have bought a 4K TV.  Gilovich says …

 

“We consume experiences directly with other people.

And after they’re gone, they’re part of the

stories that we tell to one another.”

 

And even if someone wasn’t with you when you had a particular experience, you’re much more likely to bond over both having hiked the Appalachian Trail or seeing the same show than you are over both owning Fitbits.

You’re also much less prone to negatively compare your own experiences to someone else’s than you would with material purchases. One study conducted by researchers Ryan Howell and Graham Hill found that it’s easier to feature-compare material goods (how many carats is your ring? how fast is your laptop’s CPU?) than experiences. And since it’s easier to compare, people do so.  Gilovich says …

 

“The tendency of keeping up with the Joneses tends

to be more pronounced for material goods than for

experiential purchases.  It certainly bothers us if

we’re on a vacation and see people staying in a

better hotel or flying first class. But it doesn’t

produce as much envy as when we’re

outgunned on material goods.”

 

Gilovich’s research has implications for individuals who want to maximize their happiness return on their financial investments, for employers who want to have a happier workforce, and policy-makers who want to have a happy citizenry.

 

“By shifting the investments that societies make

and the policies they pursue, they can steer

large populations to the kinds of experiential

pursuits that promote greater happiness,”

 

… write Gilovich and his coauthor, Amit Kumar, in their recent article in the academic journal Experimental Social Psychology.

If society takes their research to heart, it should mean not only a shift in how individuals spend their discretionary income, but also place an emphasis on employers giving paid vacation and governments taking care of recreational spaces.  Gilovich asks …

“As a society,

shouldn’t we be making experiences

easier for people to have?”

 •       •       •       •      •       •       •       •

Personally, my wife and I have done considerable amounts of traveling.  We’ve been to many countries and typically have artifacts from each trip.  I still enjoy seeing the artifacts, which are scattered about our home.

But the memory of those experiences is, as the research suggests, probably stronger.

Comments are closed.